Introduction
Cognition is the set of all mental abilities and processes related to knowledge, which encompasses attention, memory, judgment and evaluation, reasoning and computation, problem solving and decision making as well as comprehension and production of language. Cognitive function can be affected by several factors including emotion and sleep. Restless legs syndrome (RLS) is a sensorimotor network disorder characterized by an irresistible urge to move the legs during rest accompanied by unpleasant and disturbing sensations in the legs [1]. In RLS, cognitive dysfunction may ensue due to sleep disturbance, emotional disorders, and attention deficit which are common in RLS. There are a few unresolved issues regarding cognition in RLS. The first question should be “Is cognition really impaired in RLS?”. Although recent studies have reported that moderate to severe RLS patients may have significant cognitive deficits, primarily in executive functioning, compared with healthy control subjects [2-5], other studies found no difference in cognitive function between RLS patients and controls. Therefore, this controversy has been mentioned at the first part of this article. The next question should be “Which cognitive domain(s) are affected in those studies which revealed cognitive dysfunction in RLS?”. After these discussion, factors associated with cognitive dysfunction in RLS should be mentioned. Lastly, the effect of dopaminergic agent on cognitive dysfunction in RLS needs to be addressed. In this review, the answers for these questions will be explored. At the end of this review, sophisticated measures which might help identifying cognitive functioning in RLS such as event-related potential (ERP) studies will be mentioned.
Is Cognitive Function in RLS Really Impaired?
To date, there are ten published articles which studied cognition in RLS (Table 1). Five were community-based studies and the rest were hospital-based [3,5-8]. Five studies reported cognitive dysfunction in RLS patients [2-5,7]. Three reported no significant differences in cognitive performance between RLS patients and control subjects [6,8,9]. In these three studies, RLS patients were older and had less severe RLS in contrast to the five studies which reported cognitive dysfunction. Surprisingly, the other two studies reported that even better cognitive function was observed in patients with RLS compared to control subjects [10,11].
The discrepancies among studies may be attributable to several factors such as differences in study samples, use of different age groups (middle-aged vs. old-aged), types of neuropsychological tests administered (full batteries vs. limited to frontal function tests), and medication effects (unmedicated vs. medicated).
Which Cognitive Domains Are Affected in RLS?
Among the five studies which reported cognitive dysfunction, only one study performed comprehensive neurocognitive tests (Table 2) [7]. The other four studies included limited tests, focusing on only the frontal executive and mental flexibility tests [2-5]. All of the five studies demonstrated decreased performance in attention, frontal executive function, and mental flexibility function. In contrast, only one study revealed dysfunction in verbal and visuospatial memory, working memory, and language functions unlike the other studies, which revealed normal cognitive function in these areas [7].
Potential Factors Associated with Cognitive Dysfunction in RLS
Legs discomfort, which is aggravated by lying down to sleep in the evening, hinders beginning and maintaining of sleep in severe RLS patients. This leads RLS patients to suffer from sleep disturbance, causing chronic partial sleep loss [1,12]. Since cognitive function is affected by sleep loss, sleep deprivation due to RLS symptoms might be the cause of cognitive dysfunction in RLS patients.
Another possible mechanism of cognitive dysfunction can be explained by decreased attention due to RLS symptoms itself. Clinically, RLS patients frequently experience hyperactivity and inattention, which can mimic symptoms of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Furthermore, RLS might be associated with ADHD [13,14].
Comorbid psychiatric conditions such as depression and anxiety are common in patients with RLS [15,16]. Epidemiological studies report a 2- to 4-fold risk of a depressive disorder in patients with RLS compared to healthy controls. As depression is well known to compromise cognitive function, depression might be considered as a contributing factor to the cognitive deficit in patients with RLS. Furthermore, RLS-related sleep loss might cause depressive symptoms, and vice versa [15]. Thus, these factors may affect cognitive function in patients with RLS. Therefore, these factors should be taken into consideration in the evaluation of cognitive dysfunction in RLS (Fig. 1).
Pearson et al. reported that RLS patients show cognitive deficits, particularly in prefrontal function, which are comparable to the loss of a night’s sleep [3]. In a subsequent comparative study of RLS patients and sleep restricted controls, however, the same group found that RLS patients performed significantly better on prefrontal function tests than controls [10]. Although the authors proposed that RLS subjects might adapt to sleep loss to some extent, sleep loss alone does not appear to explain cognitive dysfunction in RLS.
Fulda et al. showed that RLS patients performed worse than controls in the area of attention and verbal fluency [2]. The International RLS Severity Scale scores were significantly associated with performance in the cancellation task. However, task performance did not correlated with subjective sleep quality and duration. A community based study reported by Celle et al. revealed that RLS patients had lower cognitive performances at Stroop and Verbal fluency tests than controls (Table 3) [4]. Correlation analysis showed that neither RLS severity score nor degree of sleep disturbances were associated with the degree of cognitive dysfunction, indicating independent effect of RLS on such cognitive tasks. Fulda et al. demonstrated no statistically significant differences in any of the neuropsychological parameters between RLS patients and controls [2]. However, comparing participants with frequent RLS symptoms to matched controls revealed decreased performance on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in RLS patients, suggesting association between cognitive dysfunction and RLS severity. Moon et al. revealed that RLS patients performed better than controls on tests of verbal memory and category word fluency [10]. RLS patients scored higher Beck depression inventory than controls. To identify whether depression had an impact on cognitive function, we compared cognitive function between patients with moderate to severe depression and those without depression. However, there was no significant differences in cognitive function between patients with moderate to severe depression and those without depression.
Effect of Dopaminergic Treatment on Cognition in RLS
Dopamine agonists are recommended as a first-line treatment in cases of moderate to severe RLS, improving primary RLS symptoms [17]. In addition, dopamine agonists improve periodic leg movements during sleep [18], depressive symptoms [19], and quality of life [20]. Two studies reported the effects of dopamine agonists on cognition in RLS subjects. Kim et al. studied sixteen drug-naïve RLS patients who were treated with pramipexole for 3 months [21]. After treatment with pramipexole, severity of RLS symptoms decreased significantly. Sleep quality and depression also improved significantly following treatment. Neuropsychological tests showed significantly improved word frequency, digit symbol coding and verbal memory upon the treatment with pramipexole. However, no significant correlations between changes in clinical variables including sleep duration, depression, and cognitive functions before and after the treatment. The other study showed that after a three-month treatment with pramipexole in twenty RLS patients, almost all cognitive investigated domains improved [7]. Improvement in these cognitive domains was associated with improvement in RLS score, quality of life, depressive and anxiety symptoms, and daytime sleepiness. However, as they did not show correlation analysis between cognitive improvement and change of RLS severity, daytime sleepiness and depression score, factors that may be associated with cognitive improvement could not be identified.
A recent study reported the effects of pramipexole on the Sternberg working memory task performance evaluated by ERP study [22]. RLS symptoms as well as sleep quality and depression were significantly improved after treatment. Reaction time and omission error was significantly reduced following treatment with pramipexole. Parietal P300 amplitude increased significantly after the treatment (Fig. 2). Increase in frontal P300 amplitude correlated significantly with improvement of sleep quality and RLS symptoms.
In summary, dopamine agonist may improve cognitive performance in RLS. However, it remains unclear whether this is a direct effect on RLS symptoms or an indirect effect of improvement of sleep duration, daytime sleepiness, or depression, or all of them.
Event-Related Potential Study in RLS
Electroencephalogram (EEG) allows non-invasive monitoring of brain processes with excellent temporal resolution. EEG during relaxed wakefulness, that is, during the resting state, reflects a particularly important state of arousal, which can be characterized by frequency analysis. ERP is a measured brain response that is direct result of a specific sensory, cognitive, or motor event [23]. An ERP study provides useful information for exploring cognitive function and non-invasive monitoring of brain processes with excellent temporal resolution and modest spatial resolution covering the whole brain. ERP measurements during tasks may provide more sensitive means of objectively assessing cognitive function. In particular, P300 has been proposed as an index of multiple cognitive processes, including attention, context updating, and processing resource allocation [24]. Thus, EEG and ERP studies might be useful for exploring cognitive functions in RLS possibly associated with attention and/or arousal state dysfunctions.
Jung et al. firstly reported abnormalities in resting EEG and ERP during visual oddball task in 17 patients with RLS compared to 13 age-matched healthy controls [25]. Waking-resting EEG showed that RLS patients had significantly higher beta activity in frontocentral regions than controls. P300 latency was significantly longer in patients, and patients had significantly lower P300 amplitudes in frontal and central locations. Further analysis revealed that gamma-band activity and phase synchrony, which is known to be associated with working memory, was significantly decreased in RLS patients compared to normal subjects, especially at frontal region. This suggests that cognitive dysfunction in RLS patients may result from less efficient cortical information processing due to the reduced interregional neuronal synchrony as well as alterations in local activity [26]. These studies support the notion that RLS patients have an underlying cognitive dysfunction.
Working memory deficits might be caused by depression, insomnia, or sleep deprivation, which are common in RLS [27,28]. Therefore, working memory deficit would be expected in RLS patients. However, most studies, except for one study [7], reported that RLS patients have no impairment in either block span backward or digit span backward test. Kim et al. recorded ERP during Sternberg working memory task in severe RLS patients [29]. The reaction time in RLS patients was significantly longer than controls over all memory load sizes. The P300 amplitude at parietal regions in RLS patients was significantly lower than that in controls regardless of memory load sizes (Fig. 3) and negatively correlated with duration of RLS history in RLS patients.
Conclusions
Considering that multiple factors are involved in the cognitive process and that different methodologies and subjects were included in various studies, there is no consensus for cognitive impairment in RLS. Cognitive function in RLS seems to be preserved assessed by neurocognitive tests. However, sophisticated methods such as ERP study may help to identify underlying cognitive dysfunction in RLS. Treatment with dopamine agonist significantly improves cognitive performance in RLS. Further studies including functional neuroimaging study are needed to confirm whether cognition in RLS patients is impaired or not.